Convolution Theorem - Functions of A Discrete Variable... Sequences

Functions of A Discrete Variable... Sequences

By similar arguments, it can be shown that the discrete convolution of sequences and is given by:



where DTFT represents the discrete-time Fourier transform.

An important special case is the circular convolution of and defined by where is a periodic summation:

It can then be shown that:


\begin{align}
x_N * y\ &=\ \scriptstyle{DTFT}^{-1} \displaystyle \big\\
&=\ \scriptstyle{DFT}^{-1} \displaystyle \big,
\end{align}

where DFT represents the discrete Fourier transform.

The proof follows from DTFT#Periodic_data, which indicates that can be written as:

The product with is thereby reduced to a discrete-frequency function:

(also using Sampling the DTFT).

The inverse DTFT is:


\begin{align}
(x_N * y)\ &=\ \int_{0}^{1} \frac{1}{N} \sum_{k=-\infty}^{\infty} \scriptstyle{DFT}\displaystyle\{x_N\}\cdot \scriptstyle{DFT}\displaystyle\{y_N\}\cdot \delta\left(f-k/N\right)\cdot e^{i 2 \pi f n} df\\
&=\ \frac{1}{N} \sum_{k=-\infty}^{\infty} \scriptstyle{DFT}\displaystyle\{x_N\}\cdot \scriptstyle{DFT}\displaystyle\{y_N\}\cdot \int_{0}^{1} \delta\left(f-k/N\right)\cdot e^{i 2 \pi f n} df\\
&=\ \frac{1}{N} \sum_{k=0}^{N-1} \scriptstyle{DFT}\displaystyle\{x_N\}\cdot \scriptstyle{DFT}\displaystyle\{y_N\}\cdot e^{i 2 \pi \frac{n}{N} k}\\
&=\ \scriptstyle{DFT}^{-1} \displaystyle \big,
\end{align}

QED.

Read more about this topic:  Convolution Theorem

Famous quotes containing the words functions of, functions, discrete and/or variable:

    When Western people train the mind, the focus is generally on the left hemisphere of the cortex, which is the portion of the brain that is concerned with words and numbers. We enhance the logical, bounded, linear functions of the mind. In the East, exercises of this sort are for the purpose of getting in tune with the unconscious—to get rid of boundaries, not to create them.
    Edward T. Hall (b. 1914)

    Adolescents, for all their self-involvement, are emerging from the self-centeredness of childhood. Their perception of other people has more depth. They are better equipped at appreciating others’ reasons for action, or the basis of others’ emotions. But this maturity functions in a piecemeal fashion. They show more understanding of their friends, but not of their teachers.
    Terri Apter (20th century)

    We have good reason to believe that memories of early childhood do not persist in consciousness because of the absence or fragmentary character of language covering this period. Words serve as fixatives for mental images. . . . Even at the end of the second year of life when word tags exist for a number of objects in the child’s life, these words are discrete and do not yet bind together the parts of an experience or organize them in a way that can produce a coherent memory.
    Selma H. Fraiberg (20th century)

    There is not so variable a thing in nature as a lady’s head-dress.
    Joseph Addison (1672–1719)